Header logo is ei


1998


no image
PET with 18fluorodeoxyglucose and hexamethylpropylene amine oxime SPECT in late whiplash syndrome

Bicik, I., Radanov, B., Schaefer, N., Dvorak, J., Blum, B., Weber, B., Burger, C., von Schulthess, G., Buck, A.

Neurology, 51, pages: 345-350, 1998 (article)

[BibTex]

1998

[BibTex]


no image
Changes of cerebral blood flow during short-term exposure to normobaric hypoxia

Buck, A., Schirlo, C., Jasinsky, V., Weber, B., Burger, C., von Schulthess, G., Koller, E., Pavlicek, V.

J Cereb Blood Flow Metab, 18, pages: 906-910, 1998 (article)

[BibTex]

[BibTex]


no image
Kernel PCA pattern reconstruction via approximate pre-images.

Schölkopf, B., Mika, S., Smola, A., Rätsch, G., Müller, K.

In 8th International Conference on Artificial Neural Networks, pages: 147-152, Perspectives in Neural Computing, (Editors: L Niklasson and M Boden and T Ziemke), Springer, Berlin, Germany, 8th International Conference on Artificial Neural Networks, 1998 (inproceedings)

[BibTex]

[BibTex]


no image
Generalization Bounds for Convex Combinations of Kernel Functions

Smola, A., Williamson, R., Schölkopf, B.

Royal Holloway College, 1998 (techreport)

[BibTex]

[BibTex]


no image
Generalization Performance of Regularization Networks and Support Vector Machines via Entropy Numbers of Compact Operators

Williamson, R., Smola, A., Schölkopf, B.

(19), NeuroCOLT, 1998, Accepted for publication in IEEE Transactions on Information Theory (techreport)

[BibTex]

[BibTex]


no image
A bootstrap method for testing hypotheses concerning psychometric functions

Hill, N., Wichmann, F.

1998 (poster)

Abstract
Whenever psychometric functions are used to evaluate human performance on some task, it is valuable to examine not only the threshold and slope values estimated from the original data, but also the expected variability in those measures. This allows psychometric functions obtained in two experimental conditions to be compared statistically. We present a method for estimating the variability of thresholds and slopes of psychometric functions. This involves a maximum-likelihood fit to the data using a three-parameter mathematical function, followed by Monte Carlo simulation using the first fit as a generating function for the simulations. The variability of the function's parameters can then be estimated (as shown by Maloney, 1990), as can the variability of the threshold value (Foster & Bischof, 1997). We will show how a simple development of this procedure can be used to test the significance of differences between (a) the thresholds, and (b) the slopes of two psychometric functions. Further, our method can be used to assess the assumptions underlying the original fit, by examining how goodness-of-fit differs in simulation from its original value. In this way data sets can be identified as being either too noisy to be generated by a binomial observer, or significantly "too good to be true." All software is written in MATLAB and is therefore compatible across platforms, with the option of accelerating performance using MATLAB's plug-in binaries, or "MEX" files.

[BibTex]


no image
Quantization Functionals and Regularized PrincipalManifolds

Smola, A., Mika, S., Schölkopf, B.

NeuroCOLT, 1998, NC2-TR-1998-028 (techreport)

[BibTex]

[BibTex]


no image
Support Vector Machines for Image Classification

Chapelle, O.

Biologische Kybernetik, Ecole Normale Superieure de Lyon, 1998 (diplomathesis)

GZIP [BibTex]

GZIP [BibTex]


no image
Support Vector methods in learning and feature extraction

Schölkopf, B., Smola, A., Müller, K., Burges, C., Vapnik, V.

Ninth Australian Conference on Neural Networks, pages: 72-78, (Editors: T. Downs, M. Frean and M. Gallagher), 1998 (talk)

[BibTex]

[BibTex]


no image
Convex Cost Functions for Support Vector Regression

Smola, A., Schölkopf, B., Müller, K.

In 8th International Conference on Artificial Neural Networks, pages: 99-104, Perspectives in Neural Computing, (Editors: L Niklasson and M Boden and T Ziemke), Springer, Berlin, Germany, 8th International Conference on Artificial Neural Networks, 1998 (inproceedings)

[BibTex]

[BibTex]


no image
Support-Vektor-Lernen

Schölkopf, B.

In Ausgezeichnete Informatikdissertationen 1997, pages: 135-150, (Editors: G Hotz and H Fiedler and P Gorny and W Grass and S Hölldobler and IO Kerner and R Reischuk), Teubner Verlag, Stuttgart, 1998 (inbook)

[BibTex]

[BibTex]


no image
Nonlinearities and the pedestal effect

Wichmann, F., Henning, G., Ploghaus, A.

Perception, 27, pages: S86, 1998 (poster)

Abstract
Psychophysical and physiological evidence suggests that luminance patterns are independently analysed in "channels" responding to different bands of spatial frequency. There are, however, interactions among stimuli falling well outside the usual estimates of channels' bandwidths (Henning, Hertz, and Broadbent, (1975). Vision Res., 15, 887-899). We examined whether the masking results of Henning et al. are consistent with independent channels. We postulated, before the channels, a point non-linearity which would introduce distortion products that might produce the observed interactions between stimuli two octaves apart in spatial frequency. Standard 2-AFC masking experiments determined whether possible distortion products of a 4.185 c/deg masking sinusoid revealed their presence through effects on the detection of a sinusoidal signal at the frequency of the second harmonic of the masker-8.37 c/deg. The signal and masker were horizontally orientated and the signal was in-phase, out-of-phase, or in quadrature with the putative second-order distortion product of the masker. Significant interactions between signal and masker were observed: for a wide range of masker contrasts, signal detection was facilitated by the masking stimulus. However, the shapes of the functions relating detection performance to masker contrast, as well as the effects of relative phase, were inconsistent with the notion that distortion products were responsible for the interactions observed.

[BibTex]

[BibTex]


no image
Funktionelle Magnetresonanztomographie in der psychopathologischen Forschung.

Spitzer, M., Kammer, T., Bellemann, M., Brix, G., Layer, B., Maier, S., Kischka, U., Gückel, F.

Fortschritte der Neurologie Psychiatrie, 66, pages: 241-258, 1998 (article)

Abstract
Mental disorders are characterised by psychopathological symptoms which correspond to functional brain states. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) is used for the non-invasive study of cerebral activation patterns in man. First of all, the neurobiological principles and presuppositions of the method are outlined. Results from the Heidelberg imaging lab on several simple sensorimotor tasks as well as higher cognitive functions, such as working and semantic memory, are then presented. Thereafter, results from preliminary fMRI studies of psychopathological symptoms are discussed, with emphasis on hallucinations, psychomotoric phenomena, emotions, as well as obsessions and compulsions. Functional MRI is limited by the physics underlying the method, as well as by practical constraints regarding its use in conjunction with mentally ill patients. Within this framework, the problems of signal-to-noise ratio, data analysis strategies, motion correction, and neurovascular coupling are considered. Because of the rapid development of the field of fMRI, maps of higher cognitive functions and their respective pathology seem to be coming within easy reach.

[BibTex]

[BibTex]


no image
Support vector regression with automatic accuracy control.

Schölkopf, B., Bartlett, P., Smola, A., Williamson, R.

In ICANN'98, pages: 111-116, Perspectives in Neural Computing, (Editors: L Niklasson and M Boden and T Ziemke), Springer, Berlin, Germany, International Conference on Artificial Neural Networks (ICANN'98), 1998 (inproceedings)

[BibTex]

[BibTex]


no image
General cost functions for support vector regression.

Smola, A., Schölkopf, B., Müller, K.

In Ninth Australian Conference on Neural Networks, pages: 79-83, (Editors: T Downs and M Frean and M Gallagher), 9th Australian Conference on Neural Networks (ACNN'98), 1998 (inproceedings)

[BibTex]

[BibTex]


no image
Asymptotically optimal choice of varepsilon-loss for support vector machines.

Smola, A., Murata, N., Schölkopf, B., Müller, K.

In 8th International Conference on Artificial Neural Networks, pages: 105-110, Perspectives in Neural Computing, (Editors: L Niklasson and M Boden and T Ziemke), Springer, Berlin, Germany, 8th International Conference on Artificial Neural Networks, 1998 (inproceedings)

[BibTex]

[BibTex]


no image
Support Vector Machine Reference Manual

Saunders, C., Stitson, M., Weston, J., Bottou, L., Schölkopf, B., Smola, A.

(CSD-TR-98-03), Department of Computer Science, Royal Holloway, University of London, 1998 (techreport)

PostScript [BibTex]

PostScript [BibTex]

1995


no image
View-based cognitive map learning by an autonomous robot

Mallot, H., Bülthoff, H., Georg, P., Schölkopf, B., Yasuhara, K.

In Proceedings International Conference on Artificial Neural Networks, vol. 2, pages: 381-386, (Editors: Fogelman-Soulié, F.), EC2, Paris, France, Conférence Internationale sur les Réseaux de Neurones Artificiels (ICANN '95), October 1995 (inproceedings)

Abstract
This paper presents a view-based approach to map learning and navigation in mazes. By means of graph theory we have shown that the view-graph is a sufficient representation for map behaviour such as path planning. A neural network for unsupervised learning of the view-graph from sequences of views is constructed. We use a modified Kohonen (1988) learning rule that transforms temporal sequence (rather than featural similarity) into connectedness. In the main part of the paper, we present a robot implementation of the scheme. The results show that the proposed network is able to support map behaviour in simple environments.

PDF [BibTex]

1995

PDF [BibTex]


no image
Extracting support data for a given task

Schölkopf, B., Burges, C., Vapnik, V.

In First International Conference on Knowledge Discovery & Data Mining (KDD-95), pages: 252-257, (Editors: UM Fayyad and R Uthurusamy), AAAI Press, Menlo Park, CA, USA, August 1995 (inproceedings)

Abstract
We report a novel possibility for extracting a small subset of a data base which contains all the information necessary to solve a given classification task: using the Support Vector Algorithm to train three different types of handwritten digit classifiers, we observed that these types of classifiers construct their decision surface from strongly overlapping small (k: 4%) subsets of the data base. This finding opens up the possibiiity of compressing data bases significantly by disposing of the data which is not important for the solution of a given task. In addition, we show that the theory allows us to predict the classifier that will have the best generalization ability, based solely on performance on the training set and characteristics of the learning machines. This finding is important for cases where the amount of available data is limited.

PDF [BibTex]

PDF [BibTex]


no image
View-Based Cognitive Mapping and Path Planning

Schölkopf, B., Mallot, H.

Adaptive Behavior, 3(3):311-348, January 1995 (article)

Abstract
This article presents a scheme for learning a cognitive map of a maze from a sequence of views and movement decisions. The scheme is based on an intermediate representation called the view graph, whose nodes correspond to the views whereas the labeled edges represent the movements leading from one view to another. By means of a graph theoretical reconstruction method, the view graph is shown to carry complete information on the topological and directional structure of the maze. Path planning can be carried out directly in the view graph without actually performing this reconstruction. A neural network is presented that learns the view graph during a random exploration of the maze. It is based on an unsupervised competitive learning rule translating temporal sequence (rather than similarity) of views into connectedness in the network. The network uses its knowledge of the topological and directional structure of the maze to generate expectations about which views are likely to be encountered next, improving the view-recognition performance. Numerical simulations illustrate the network's ability for path planning and the recognition of views degraded by random noise. The results are compared to findings of behavioral neuroscience.

Web DOI [BibTex]

Web DOI [BibTex]


no image
Suppression and creation of chaos in a periodically forced Lorenz system.

Franz, MO., Zhang, MH.

Physical Review, E 52, pages: 3558-3565, 1995 (article)

Abstract
Periodic forcing is introduced into the Lorenz model to study the effects of time-dependent forcing on the behavior of the system. Such a nonautonomous system stays dissipative and has a bounded attracting set which all trajectories finally enter. The possible kinds of attracting sets are restricted to periodic orbits and strange attractors. A large-scale survey of parameter space shows that periodic forcing has mainly three effects in the Lorenz system depending on the forcing frequency: (i) Fixed points are replaced by oscillations around them; (ii) resonant periodic orbits are created both in the stable and the chaotic region; (iii) chaos is created in the stable region near the resonance frequency and in periodic windows. A comparison to other studies shows that part of this behavior has been observed in simulations of higher truncations and real world experiments. Since very small modulations can already have a considerable effect, this suggests that periodic processes such as annual or diurnal cycles should not be omitted even in simple climate models.

[BibTex]

[BibTex]


no image
A New Method for Constructing Artificial Neural Networks

Vapnik, V., Burges, C., Schölkopf, B.

AT & T Bell Laboratories, 1995 (techreport)

[BibTex]

[BibTex]


no image
Image segmentation from motion: just the loss of high-spatial-frequency content ?

Wichmann, F., Henning, G.

Perception, 24, pages: S19, 1995 (poster)

Abstract
The human contrast sensitivity function (CSF) is bandpass for stimuli of low temporal frequency but, for moving stimuli, results in a low-pass CSF with large high spatial-frequency losses. Thus the high spatial-frequency content of images moving on the retina cannot be seen; motion perception could be facilitated by, or even be based on, the selective loss of high spatial-frequency content. 2-AFC image segmentation experiments were conducted with segmentation based on motion or on form. In the latter condition, the form difference mirrored that produced by moving stimuli. This was accomplished by generating stimulus elements which were spectrally either broadband or low-pass. For the motion used, the spectral difference between static broadband and static low-pass elements matched the spectral difference between moving and static broadband elements. On the hypothesis that segmentation from motion is based on the detection of regions devoid of high spatial-frequencies, both tasks should be similarly difficult for human observers. However, neither image segmentation (nor, incidentally, motion detection) was sensitive to the high spatial-frequency content of the stimuli. Thus changes in perceptual form produced by moving stimuli appear not to be used as a cue for image segmentation.

[BibTex]